The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt Flashcards

1
Q

Wisdom is now so cheap and abundant that it floods over us from calendar pages, tea bags, bottle caps,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Shakespeare: “There is nothing either good or bad, but thinking makes it so. ”1.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Helping people find happiness and meaning is precisely the goal of the new field of positive psychology,2.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

The mind is divided into parts that sometimes conflict. Like a rider on the back of an elephant, the conscious, reasoning part of the mind has only limited control of what the elephant does.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Shakespeare’s, about how “thinking makes it so. ” (Or, as Buddha4 said, “Our life is the creation of our mind. ”) But we can improve this ancient idea today by explaining why most people’s minds have a bias toward seeing threats and engaging in useless worry.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Reciprocity is the most important tool for getting along with people,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

One is that happiness comes from getting what you want, but we all know (and research confirms) that such happiness is short-lived. A more promising hypothesis is that happiness comes from within and cannot be obtained by making the world conform to your desires.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Changing your mind is usually a more effective response to frustration than is changing the world.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

For what the flesh desires is opposed to the Spirit, and what the Spirit desires is opposed to the flesh; for these are opposed to each other, to prevent you from doing what you want. —ST. PAUL, GALATIANS 5:171.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

If Passion drives, let Reason hold the Reins. —BENJAMIN FRANKLIN.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Human thinking depends on metaphor. We understand new or complex things in relation to things we already know. 3.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Buddha, for example, compared the mind to a wild elephant: In days gone by this mind of mine used to stray wherever selfish desire or lust or pleasure would lead it. Today this mind does not stray and is under the harmony of control, even as a wild elephant is controlled by the trainer. 4.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Sigmund Freud offered us a related model 2,300 years later. 6 Freud said that the mind is divided into three parts: the ego (the conscious, rational self); the superego (the conscience, a sometimes too rigid commitment to the rules of society); and the id (the desire for pleasure, lots of it, sooner rather than later).

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

The metaphor I use when I lecture on Freud is to think of the mind as a horse and buggy (a Victorian chariot) in which the driver (the ego) struggles frantically to control a hungry, lustful, and disobedient horse (the id) while the driver’s father (the superego) sits in the back seat lecturing the driver on what he is doing wrong.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

For Freud, the goal of psychoanalysis was to escape this pitiful state by strengthening the ego, thus giving it more control over the id and more independence from the superego.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

The last third of the century: Social psychologists created “information processing” theories to explain everything from prejudice to friendship. Economists created “rational choice” models to explain why people do what they do. The social sciences were uniting under the idea that people are rational agents who set goals and pursue them intelligently by using the information and resources at their disposal.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

I am dragged along by a strange new force. Desire and reason are pulling in different directions. I see the right way and approve it, but follow the wrong. 7.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

I was a rider on the back of an elephant. I’m holding the reins in my hands, and by pulling one way or the other I can tell the elephant to turn, to stop, or to go. I can direct things, but only when the elephant doesn’t have desires of his own. When the elephant really wants to do something, I’m no match for him.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Trying to improve the workings of the head brain can directly interfere with those of the gut brain.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Ancient Indian theories in which the abdomen contains the three lower chakras—energy centers corresponding to the colon/anus, sexual organs, and gut.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

The corpus callosum is the largest single bundle of nerves in the entire body, so it must be doing something important. Indeed it is: It allows the two halves of the brain to communicate and coordinate their activity.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

The brain divides its processing of the world into its two hemispheres—left and right. The left hemisphere takes in information from the right half of the world (that is, it receives nerve transmissions from the right arm and leg, the right ear, and the left half of each retina, which receives light from the right half of the visual field) and sends out commands to move the limbs on the right side of the body.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

The left hemisphere is specialized for language processing and analytical tasks. In visual tasks, it is better at noticing details. The right hemisphere is better at processing patterns in space, including that all-important pattern, the face. (This is the origin of popular and oversimplified ideas about artists being “right-brained” and scientists being “left-brained”).

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

These dramatic splits of the mind are caused by rare splits of the brain. Normal people are not split-brained. Yet the split-brain studies were important in psychology because they showed in such an eerie way that the mind is a confederation of modules capable of working independently and even, sometimes, at cross-purposes.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Gazzaniga’s “interpreter module” is, essentially, the rider.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

Perhaps the frontal cortex is the seat of reason: It is Plato’s charioteer; it is St. Paul’s Spirit. And it has taken over control, though not perfectly, from the more primitive limbic system—Plato’s bad horse, St. Paul’s flesh.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

The limbic system underlies many of our basic animal instincts. 14.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

When people suffer damage to the frontal cortex, they sometimes show an increase in sexual and aggressive behavior because the frontal cortex plays an important role in suppressing or inhibiting behavioral impulses.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

A brain scan found that an enormous tumor in his frontal cortex was squeezing everything else, preventing the frontal cortex from doing its job of inhibiting inappropriate behavior and thinking about consequences.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

In fact, the frontal cortex enabled a great expansion of emotionality in humans. The lower third of the prefrontal cortex is called the orbitofrontal cortex because it is the part of the brain just above the eyes (orbit is the Latin term for the eye socket). This region of the cortex has grown especially large in humans and other primates and is one of the most consistently active areas of the brain during emotional reactions. 16.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

The orbitofrontal cortex therefore appears to be a better candidate for the id, or for St. Paul’s flesh, than for the superego or the Spirit.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

When they look out at the world and think, “What should I do now?” they see dozens of choices but lack immediate internal feelings of like or dislike. They must examine the pros and cons of every choice with their reasoning, but in the absence of feeling they see little reason to pick one or the other.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

Human rationality depends critically on sophisticated emotionality. It is only because our emotional brains works so well that our reasoning can work at all.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
34
Q

Reason and emotion must both work together to create intelligent behavior, but emotion (a major part of the elephant) does most of the work. When the neocortex came along, it made the rider possible, but it made the elephant much smarter, too.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
35
Q

There are really two processing systems at work in the mind at all times: controlled processes and automatic processes.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
36
Q

If half the sentences you unscrambled contained words related to rudeness (such as bother, brazen, aggressively), you will probably interrupt the experimenter within a minute or two to say, “Hey, I’m finished. What should I do now?” But if you unscrambled sentences in which the rude words were swapped with words related to politeness (“they her respect see usually”), the odds are you’ll just sit there meekly and wait until the experimenter acknowledges you—ten minutes from now.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
37
Q

Some part of the mind does see the words, and it sets in motion behaviors that psychologists can measure.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
38
Q

Controlled processing is limited—we can think consciously about one thing at a time only—but automatic processes run in parallel and can handle many tasks at once. If the mind performs hundreds of operations each second, all but one of them must be handled automatically.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
39
Q

Controlled processing requires language. You can have bits and pieces of thought through images, but to plan something complex, to weigh the pros and cons of different paths, or to analyze the causes of past successes and failures, you need words.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
40
Q

Nobody knows how long ago human beings developed language, but most estimates range from around 2 million years ago, when hominid brains became much bigger, to as recently as 40,000 years ago, the time of cave paintings and other artifacts that reveal unmistakably modern human minds. 23.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
41
Q

Automatic processes, on the other hand, have been through thousands of product cycles and are nearly perfect.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
42
Q

Evolution never looks ahead. It can’t plan the best way to travel from point A to point B. Instead, small changes to existing forms arise (by genetic mutation), and spread within a population to the extent that they help organisms respond more effectively to current conditions.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
43
Q

The rider evolved to serve to the elephant.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
44
Q

It is no accident that we find the carnal pleasures so rewarding. Our brains, like rat brains, are wired so that food and sex give us little bursts of dopamine,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
45
Q

The automatic system has its finger on the dopamine release button. The controlled system, in contrast, is better seen as an advisor. It’s a rider placed on the elephant’s back to help the elephant make better choices.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
46
Q

The rider can see farther into the future, and the rider can learn valuable information by talking to other riders or by reading maps, but the rider cannot order the elephant around against its will.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
47
Q

Scottish philosopher David Hume was closer to the truth than was Plato when he said, “Reason is, and ought only to be the slave of the passions, and can never pretend to any other office than to serve and obey them. ”26.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
48
Q

In sum, the rider is an advisor or servant; not a king, president, or charioteer with a firm grip on the reins.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
49
Q

The elephant and the rider each have their own intelligence, and when they work together well they enable the unique brilliance of human beings. But they don’t always work together well.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
50
Q

Mischel discovers that the number of seconds you waited to ring the bell in 1970 predicts not only what your parents say about you as a teenager but also the likelihood that you were admitted to a top university. Children who were able to overcome stimulus control and delay gratification for a few extra minutes in 1970 were better able to resist temptation as teenagers, to focus on their studies, and to control themselves when things didn’t go the way they wanted. 27.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
51
Q

A large part of it was strategy—the ways that children used their limited mental control to shift attention. In later studies, Mischel discovered that the successful children were those who looked away from the temptation or were able to think about other enjoyable activities. 28 These thinking skills are an aspect of emotional intelligence—an ability to understand and regulate one’s own feelings and desires. 29.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
52
Q

An emotionally intelligent person has a skilled rider who knows how to distract and coax the elephant without having to engage in a direct contest of wills.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
53
Q

It’s hard for the controlled system to beat the automatic system by willpower alone; like a tired muscle,30 the former soon wears down and caves in, but the latter runs automatically, effortlessly, and endlessly.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
54
Q

Buddhism, for example, in an effort to break people’s carnal attachment to their own (and others’) flesh, developed methods of meditating on decaying corpses. 31 By choosing to stare at something that revolts the automatic system, the rider can begin to change what the elephant will want in the future.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
55
Q

The imp works hard to suggest the most inappropriate things I could possibly say.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
56
Q

The moment one stops trying to suppress a thought, the thought comes flooding in and becomes even harder to banish. In other words, Wegner creates minor obsessions in his lab by instructing people not to obsess. Wegner explains this effect as an “ironic process” of mental control. 32.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
57
Q

When controlled processing tries to influence thought (“Don’t think about a white bear!”), it sets up an explicit goal. And whenever one pursues a goal, a part of the mind automatically monitors progress,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
58
Q

But because controlled processes tire quickly, eventually the inexhaustible automatic processes run unopposed, conjuring up herds of white bears. Thus, the attempt to remove an unpleasant thought can guarantee it a place on your frequent-play list of mental ruminations.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
59
Q

Automatic processes generate thousands of thoughts and images every day, often through random association. The ones that get stuck are the ones that particularly shock us, the ones we try to suppress or deny. The reason we suppress them is not that we know, deep down, that they’re true (although some may be), but that they are scary or shameful. Yet once we have tried and failed to suppress them, they can become the sorts of obsessive thoughts that make us believe in.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
60
Q

If you listen closely to moral arguments, you can sometimes hear something surprising: that it is really the elephant holding the reins, guiding the rider. It is the elephant who decides what is good or bad, beautiful or ugly. Gut feelings, intuitions, and snap judgments happen constantly and automatically.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
61
Q

The whole universe is change and life itself is but what you deem it. —MARCUS AURELIUS1.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
62
Q

What we are today comes from our thoughts of yesterday, and our present thoughts build our life of tomorrow: our life is the creation of our mind. —BUDDHA2.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
63
Q

Events in the world affect us only through our interpretations of them, so if we can control our interpretations, we can control our world.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
64
Q

Dale Carnegie, writing in 1944, called the last eight words of the Aurelius quote “eight words that can transform your life. ”3.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
65
Q

Often a moment comes when a person consumed by years of resentment, pain, and anger realizes that her father (for example) didn’t directly hurt her when he abandoned the family; all he did was move out of the house. His action was morally wrong, but the pain came from her reactions to the event, and if she can change those reactions, she can leave behind twenty years of pain and perhaps even get to know her father.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
66
Q

“Nothing is miserable unless you think it so; and on the other hand, nothing brings happiness unless you are content with it. ”7.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
67
Q

Epiphanies can be life-altering,8 but most fade in days or weeks. The rider can’t just decide to change and then order the elephant to go along with the program. Lasting change can come only by retraining the elephant, and that’s hard to do.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
68
Q

When pop psychology programs are successful in helping people, which they sometimes are, they succeed not because of the initial moment of insight but because they find ways to alter people’s behavior over the following months. They keep people involved with the program long enough to retrain the elephant.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
69
Q

The most important words in the elephant’s language are “like” and “dislike,” or “approach” and “withdraw. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
70
Q

Your negative evaluation of boredom has been facilitated, or “primed,” by your tiny flash of negativity toward fear. If, however, the word following fear is garden, you would take longer to say that garden is good, because of the time it takes for your like-o-meter to shift from bad to good. 9 The discovery of affective priming.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
71
Q

Researchers have found that Americans of all ages, classes, and political affiliations react with a flash of negativity to black faces or to other images and words associated with African-American culture. 10.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
72
Q

(You can test your own elephant at: www. Projectimplicit. Com. ) Even many African Americans show this implicit prejudice,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
73
Q

Brett Pelham,11 who has discovered that one’s like-o-meter is triggered by one’s own name.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
74
Q

Whenever you see or hear a word that resembles your name, a little flash of pleasure biases you toward thinking the thing is good.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
75
Q

People named Dennis or Denise are slightly more likely than people with other names to become dentists. Men named Lawrence and women named Laurie are more likely to become lawyers.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
76
Q

People are slightly more likely to marry people whose names sound like their own, even if the similarity is just sharing a first initial.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
77
Q

The unsettling implication of Pelham’s work is that the three biggest decisions most of us make—what to do with our lives, where to live, and whom to marry—can all be influenced (even if only slightly) by something as trivial as the sound of a name.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
78
Q

Clinical psychologists sometimes say that two kinds of people seek therapy: those who need tightening, and those who need loosening.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
79
Q

For most people, the elephant sees too many things as bad and not enough as good.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
80
Q

The cost of missing the sign of a nearby predator, however, can be catastrophic. Game over, end of the line for those genes.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
81
Q

Bad is stronger than good. Responses to threats and unpleasantness are faster, stronger, and harder to inhibit than responses to opportunities and pleasures. This principle, called “negativity bias,”13.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
82
Q

It takes at least five good or constructive actions to make up for the damage done by one critical or destructive act. 14.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
83
Q

Design principle of animal life: Opposing systems push against each other to reach a balance point, but the balance point is adjustable.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
84
Q

The sympathetic system prepares your body for “fight or flight” and the parasympathetic system calms you down.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
85
Q

One reason the withdrawal system is so quick and compelling is that it gets first crack at all incoming information. All neural impulses from the eyes and ears go first to the thalamus, a kind of central switching station in the brain. From the thalamus, neural impulses are sent out to special sensory processing areas in the cortex; and from those areas, information is relayed to the frontal cortex, where it is integrated with other higher mental processes and your ongoing stream of consciousness.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
86
Q

But because neural impulses move only at about thirty meters per second, this fairly long path, including decision time, could easily take a second or two. It’s easy to see why a neural shortcut would be advantageous, and the amygdala is that shortcut.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
87
Q

Bad is stronger and faster than good. The elephant reacts before the rider even sees the snake on the path.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
88
Q

The amygdala: Not only does it reach down to the brainstem to trigger a response to danger but it reaches up to the frontal cortex to change your thinking. It shifts the entire brain over to a withdrawal orientation.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
89
Q

Genes make at least some contribution to nearly every trait.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
90
Q

Genes are not blueprints specifying the structure of a person; they are better thought of as recipes for producing a person over many years. 26.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
91
Q

Happiness is one of the most highly heritable aspects of personality. Twin studies generally show that from 50 percent to 80 percent of all the variance among people in their average levels of happiness can be explained by differences in their genes rather than in their life experiences. 28.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
92
Q

A person’s average or typical level of happiness is that person’s “affective style. ” (“Affect” refers to the felt or experienced part of emotion. ) Your affective style reflects the everyday balance of power between your approach system and your withdrawal system,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
93
Q

You can change your affective style too—but again, you can’t do it by sheer force of will. You have to do something that will change your repertoire of available thoughts. Here are three of the best methods for doing so: meditation, cognitive therapy, and Prozac. All three are effective because they work on the elephant.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
94
Q

The goal of meditation is to change automatic thought processes, thereby taming the elephant.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
95
Q

Disrespect hurts more on average than respect feels good.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
96
Q

For Buddha, attachments are like a game of roulette in which someone else spins the wheel and the game is rigged: The more you play, the more you lose. The only way to win is to step away from the table. And the only way to step away, to make yourself not react to the ups and downs of life, is to meditate and tame the mind. Although you give up the pleasures of winning, you also give up the larger pains of losing.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
97
Q

Buddha said: “When a man knows the solitude of silence, and feels the joy of quietness, he is then free from fear and sin. ”35.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
98
Q

He mapped out the distorted thought processes characteristic of depressed people and trained his patients to catch and challenge these thoughts.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
99
Q

Cognitive therapy,36 one of the most effective treatments available for depression, anxiety, and many other problems.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
100
Q

We often use reasoning not to find the truth but to invent arguments to support our deep and intuitive beliefs (residing in the elephant).

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
101
Q

Depressed people are convinced in their hearts of three related beliefs, known as Beck’s “cognitive triad” of depression. These are: “I’m no good,” “My world is bleak,” and “My future is hopeless. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
102
Q

Depressed people are caught in a feedback loop in which distorted thoughts cause negative feelings, which then distort thinking further.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
103
Q

A big part of cognitive therapy is training clients to catch their thoughts, write them down, name the distortions, and then find alternative and more accurate ways of thinking.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
104
Q

Cognitive therapy works because it teaches the rider how to train the elephant rather than how to defeat it directly in an argument.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
105
Q

You can’t win a tug of war with an angry or fearful elephant, but you can—by gradual shaping of the sort the behaviorists talked about—change your automatic thoughts and, in the process, your affective style.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
106
Q

Many therapists combine cognitive therapy with techniques borrowed directly from behaviorism to create what is now called “cognitive behavioral therapy. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
107
Q

If you have frequent automatic negative thoughts about yourself, your world, or your future, and if these thoughts contribute to chronic feelings of anxiety or despair, then you might find a good fit with cognitive behavioral therapy. 40.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
108
Q

Prozac was the first member of a class of drugs known as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, or SSRIs.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
109
Q

Prozac gets into the synapses (the gaps between neurons), but it is selective in affecting only synapses that use serotonin as their neurotransmitter. Once in the synapses, Prozac inhibits the reuptake process—the normal process in which a neuron that has just released serotonin into the synapse then sucks it back up into itself, to be released again at the next neural pulse. The net result is that a brain on Prozac has more serotonin in certain synapses, so those neurons fire more often.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
110
Q

Prozac turns out to be just about as effective as cognitive therapy—sometimes a little more, sometimes a little less—but it’s so much easier than therapy.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
111
Q

Kramer coined the term “cosmetic psychopharmacology,” for Prozac seemed to promise that psychiatrists could shape and perfect minds just as plastic surgeons shape and perfect bodies.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

112
Q

Which of these two phrases rings truest to you: “Be all that you can be” or “This above all, to thine own self be true. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

113
Q

Horror fascinates me, particularly when there is no victim.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

114
Q

My research indicates that a small set of innate moral intuitions guide and constrain the world’s many moralities,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

115
Q

Something is indeed lost when psychiatrists no longer listen to their patients as people, but rather as a car mechanic would listen to an engine, looking only for clues about which knob to adjust next.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

116
Q

Once you know why change is so hard, you can drop the brute force method and take a more psychologically sophisticated approach to self-improvement.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

117
Q

Zigong asked: “Is there any single word that could guide one’s entire life?” The master said: “Should it not be reciprocity? What you do not wish for yourself, do not do to others. ” —ANALECTS OF CONFUCIUS1.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

118
Q

That which is hateful to you, do not do to your fellow; this, in a few words, is the entire Torah; all the rest is but an elaboration of this one, central point. —RABBI HILLEL, 1ST CENT. BCE2.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

119
Q

Reciprocity is a deep instinct; it is the basic currency of social life.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

120
Q

Animals that live in large peaceful societies seem to violate the laws of evolution (such as competition and survival of the fittest), but only until you learn a bit more about evolution. Ultrasociality4—living in large cooperative societies in which hundreds or thousands of individuals reap the benefits of an extensive division of labor—evolved independently at least four times in the animal kingdom: among hymenoptera (ants, bees, and wasps); termites; naked mole rats; and humans.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

121
Q

For all the nonhuman ultrasocial species, that feature was the genetics of kin altruism.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

122
Q

The only way to “win” at the game of evolution is to leave surviving copies of your genes. Yet not just your children carry copies of your genes. Your siblings are just as closely related to you (50 percent shared genes) as your children; your nephews and nieces share a quarter of your genes, and your cousins one eighth. In a strictly Darwinian calculation, whatever cost you would… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

123
Q

Shared genes equals shared… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

124
Q

Kin altruism explains only how groups of a few dozen, or perhaps a hundred, animals can work together. Out of a flock of thousands, only a small percentage would be close enough to be worth taking risks for. The rest would be competitors, in the Darwinian sense. Here’s where the ancestors of bees, termites, and mole rats took the common mechanism of kin altruism, which makes many species sociable, and parlayed it6 into the foundation of their uncommon ultrasociality: They are all siblings. Those species each evolved a reproduction system in which a single queen produces all the children, and nearly all the children are… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

125
Q

If everyone around you is your sibling, and if the survival of your genes depends on the survival of your queen, selfishness becomes genetic suicide. These ultrasocial species display levels of cooperation and… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

126
Q

Some ants, for example, spend their lives hanging from the top of a tunnel, offering their abdomens for use as food… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

127
Q

The ultrasocial animals evolved into a state of ultrakinship, which led… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

128
Q

We humans also try to extend the reach of kin altruism by using fictitious kinship names for nonrelatives, as when children are encouraged to call their parents’ friends Uncle Bob and Aunt Sarah. Indeed, the mafia is known as “the family,” and the very idea of a godfather… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

129
Q

The human mind finds kinship deeply appealing, and kin altruism surely underlies the… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

130
Q

What would you do if you received a Christmas card from a complete stranger? This actually happened in a study in which a psychologist sent Christmas cards to people at random. … Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

131
Q

Influence,9 Robert… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

132
Q

People have a mindless, automatic reciprocity reflex. Like other animals, we will perform certain behaviors when the world presents… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

133
Q

Cialdini sees human reciprocity as a similar ethological reflex: a person receives a favor from an acquaintance and wants to repay the favor. The person will even repay an empty favor from a stranger,… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

134
Q

Play tit for tat. Do to others what they do unto you. Specifically, the tit-for-tat strategy is to be nice on the first round of interaction; but after that, do to your partner whatever… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

135
Q

Tit for tat takes us way beyond kin altruism. It opens the possibility of forming cooperative relationships with strangers. Most interactions among animals (other than close kin) are zero-sum games: One animal’s gain is the other’s loss. But life is full of situations in which cooperation would expand the pie… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

136
Q

Vampire bats, for example, will regurgitate blood from a successful night of bloodsucking into the mouth of an unsuccessful and genetically unrelated peer. Such behavior seems to violate the spirit of Darwinian competition, except that the bats keep track of who has helped… Some highlights have been hidden or truncated due to export limits.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

137
Q

Vengeance and gratitude are moral sentiments that amplify and enforce tit for tat. Vengeful and grateful feelings appear to have evolved precisely because they are such useful tools for helping individuals create cooperative relationships, thereby reaping the gains from non-zero-sum games. 13.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

138
Q

A species equipped with vengeance and gratitude responses can support larger and more cooperative social groups because the payoff to cheaters is reduced by the costs they bear in making enemies. 14 Conversely, the benefits of generosity are increased because one gains friends.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

139
Q

Tit for tat appears to be built into human nature as a set of moral emotions that make us want to return favor for favor, insult for insult, tooth for tooth, and eye for eye.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

140
Q

But the economists were wrong about you both. In real life, nobody offers one dollar, and around half of all people offer ten dollars. But what would you do if your partner offered you seven dollars? Or five? Or three? Most people would accept the seven dollars, but not the three. Most people are willing to pay a few dollars, but not seven, to punish the selfish partner.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

141
Q

What parts of the brain were more active when people were given unfair offers. One of the three areas that differed most (when comparing responses to unfair vs. Fair offers) was the frontal insula, an area of the cortex on the frontal underside of the brain. The frontal insula is known to be active during most negative or unpleasant emotional states, particularly anger and disgust. Another area was the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, just behind the sides of the forehead, known to be active during reasoning and calculation.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

142
Q

Gratitude and vengefulness are big steps on the road that led to human ultrasociality,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

143
Q

An individual who had gratitude without vengefulness would be an easy mark for exploitation, and a vengeful and ungrateful individual would quickly alienate all potential cooperative partners.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

144
Q

Gossip is another key piece in the puzzle of how humans became ultrasocial. It might also be the reason we have such large heads.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

145
Q

Human brains grow so large that human beings must be born prematurely18.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

146
Q

Maps brain size onto social group size. Robin Dunbar19 has demonstrated that within a given group of vertebrate species—primates, carnivores, ungulates, birds, reptiles, or fish—the logarithm of the brain size is almost perfectly proportional to the logarithm of the social group size.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

147
Q

All over the animal kingdom, brains grow to manage larger and larger groups. Social animals are smart animals.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

148
Q

100 to 150 is the “natural” group size within which people can know just about everyone directly, by name and face, and know how each person is related to everybody else.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

149
Q

Dunbar suggests that language evolved as a replacement for physical grooming. 20 Language allows small groups of people to bond quickly and to learn from each other about the bonds of others. Dunbar notes that people do in fact use language primarily to talk about other people—to.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

150
Q

In our ultrasocial species, success is largely a matter of playing the social game well. It’s not what you know, it’s who you know. In short, Dunbar proposes that language evolved because it enabled gossip.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

151
Q

Dunbar’s point: We are motivated to pass on information to our friends;.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

152
Q

Your friend’s reciprocity reflex kicks in and she feels a slight pressure to return the favor.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

153
Q

Gossip elicits gossip, and it enables us to keep track of everyone’s reputation without having to witness their good and bad deeds personally. Gossip creates a non-zero-sum game because it costs us nothing to give each other information, yet we both benefit by receiving information.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

154
Q

Gossip is overwhelmingly critical, and it is primarily about the moral and social violations of others.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

155
Q

People do occasionally tell stories about the good deeds of others, but such stories are only one tenth as common as stories about transgressions.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

156
Q

When people pass along high-quality (“juicy”) gossip, they feel more powerful, they have a better shared sense of what is right and what’s wrong, and they feel more closely connected to their gossip partners.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

157
Q

A second study revealed that most people hold negative views of gossip and gossipers, even though almost everyone gossips.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

158
Q

In a gossipy world, we don’t just feel vengeance and gratitude toward those who hurt or help us; we feel pale but still instructive flashes of contempt and anger toward people whom we might not even know.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

159
Q

Gossip is a policeman and a teacher. Without it, there would be chaos and ignorance. 22.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

160
Q

Many species reciprocate, but only humans gossip, and much of what we gossip about is the value of other people as partners for reciprocal relationships.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

161
Q

As long as everyone plays tit-for-tat augmented by gratitude, vengeance, and gossip, the whole system should work beautifully. (It rarely does, however, because of our self-serving biases and massive hypocrisy.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

162
Q

Reciprocity is like a magic wand that can clear your way through the jungle of social life.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

163
Q

Robert Cialdini spent years studying the dark arts of social influence:.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

164
Q

Cialdini describes six principles that salespeople use against us, but the most basic of all is reciprocity.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

165
Q

If you get something for nothing, part of you may be pleased, but part of you (part of the elephant—automatic processes) moves your hand to your wallet to give something back.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

166
Q

Students were first asked whether they would volunteer to work for two hours a week for two years with juvenile delinquents. All said no, but when the experimenter then asked about the day trip to the zoo, 50 percent said yes. 26 Concession leads to concession.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

167
Q

In financial bargaining, too, people who stake out an extreme first position and then move toward the middle end up doing better than those who state a more reasonable first position and then hold fast. 27 And the extreme offer followed by concession doesn’t just get you a better price, it gets you a happier partner (or victim): She is more likely to honor the agreement because she feels that she had more influence on the outcome. The very process of give and take creates a feeling of partnership, even in the person being taken.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

168
Q

Reciprocity is an all-purpose relationship tonic. Used properly, it strengthens, lengthens, and rejuvenates social ties. It works so well in part because the elephant is a natural mimic.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

169
Q

When we interact with someone we like, we have a slight tendency to copy their every move, automatically and unconsciously. 28.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

170
Q

But it’s not just that we mimic those we like; we like those who mimic us. People who are subtly mimicked are then more helpful and agreeable toward their mimicker, and even toward others. 29 Waitresses who mimic their customers get larger tips. 30.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

171
Q

A theme of the rest of this book is that humans are partially hive creatures, like bees, yet in the modern world we spend nearly all our time outside of the hive. Reciprocity, like love, reconnects us with others.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

172
Q

Why do you see the speck in your neighbor’s eye, but do not notice the log in your own eye?. . . You hypocrite, first take the log out of your own eye, and then you will see clearly to take the speck out of your neighbor’s eye. —MATTHEW 7:3-5.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

173
Q

There is a special pleasure in the irony of a moralist brought down for the very moral failings he has condemned.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

174
Q

The first two set the pattern, and the third violates it. With hypocrisy, the hypocrite’s preaching is the setup, the hypocritical action is the punch line.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

175
Q

Stories about the moral failings of others are among the most common kinds of gossip,3.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

176
Q

In these games, which are intended to be simple models of the game of life, no strategy ever beats tit for tat. 4 In the long run and across a variety of environments, it pays to cooperate while remaining vigilant to the danger of being cheated.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

177
Q

In real life, however, you don’t react to what someone did; you react only to what you think she did, and the gap between action and perception is bridged by the art of impression management.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

178
Q

If life itself is but what you deem it, then why not focus your efforts on persuading others to believe that you are a virtuous and trustworthy cooperator?.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

179
Q

Human beings evolved to play the game of life in a Machiavellian way. 6 The Machiavellian version of tit for tat, for example, is to do all you can to cultivate the reputation of a trustworthy yet vigilant partner, whatever the reality may be.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

180
Q

The simplest way to cultivate a reputation for being fair is to really be fair, but life and psychology experiments sometimes force us to choose between appearance and reality.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

181
Q

People who think they are particularly moral are in fact more likely to “do the right thing” and flip the coin, but when the coin flip comes out against them, they find a way to ignore it and follow their own self-interest. Batson called this tendency to value the appearance of morality over the reality “moral hypocrisy. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

182
Q

When people were forced to think about fairness and could see themselves cheating, they stopped doing it. As Jesus and Buddha said in the opening epigraphs of this chapter, it is easy to spot a cheater when our eyes are looking outward, but hard when looking inward.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

183
Q

Though you see the seven defects of others, we do not see our own ten defects. (Japanese proverb)8.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

184
Q

Most people think they are good people and that their actions are motivated by good reasons.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

185
Q

As Robert Wright put it in his masterful book The Moral Animal, “Human beings are a species splendid in their array of moral equipment, tragic in their propensity to misuse it, and pathetic in their constitutional ignorance of the misuse. ”10.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

186
Q

Curing hypocrisy is much harder because part of the problem is that we don’t believe there’s a problem.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

187
Q

People are skilled at finding reasons to support their gut feelings: The rider acts like a lawyer whom the elephant has hired to represent it in the court of public opinion.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

188
Q

Our inner lawyer works in the same way, but, somehow, we actually believe the stories he makes up.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

189
Q

Studies of everyday reasoning show that the elephant is not an inquisitive client.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

190
Q

People who are told that they have performed poorly on a test of social intelligence think extra hard to find reasons to discount the test; people who are asked to read a study showing that one of their habits—such as drinking coffee—is unhealthy think extra hard to find flaws in the study, flaws that people who don’t drink coffee don’t notice. Over and over again, studies show that people set out on a cognitive mission to bring back reasons to support their preferred belief or action.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

191
Q

Franklin concluded: “So convenient a thing is it to be a reasonable creature, since it enables one to find or make a reason for every thing one has a mind to do. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

192
Q

I don’t want to blame everything on the lawyer. The lawyer is, after all, the rider—your conscious, reasoning self; and he is taking orders from the elephant—your automatic and unconscious self. The two are in cahoots to win at the game of life by playing Machiavellian tit for tat, and both are in denial about it.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

193
Q

The consistent finding of psychological research is that we are fairly accurate in our perceptions of others. It’s our self-perceptions that are distorted because we look at ourselves in a rose-colored mirror.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

194
Q

When Americans and Europeans are asked to rate themselves on virtues, skills, or other desirable traits (including intelligence, driving ability, sexual skills, and ethics), a large majority say they are above average. 15 (This effect is weaker in East Asian countries, and may not exist in Japan. )16.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

195
Q

We judge others by their behavior, but we think we have special information about ourselves—we know what we are “really like” inside, so we can easily find ways to explain away our selfish acts and cling to the illusion that we are better than others.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

196
Q

In a study of 1 million American high school students, 70 percent thought they were above average on leadership ability, but only 2 percent thought they were below average.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

197
Q

(College professors are less wise than high school students in this respect—94 percent of us think we do above-average work. )19.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

198
Q

But when there is little room for ambiguity—how tall are you? how good are you at juggling?—people tend to be much more modest.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

199
Q

As with other kinds of social comparison, ambiguity allows us to set up the comparison in ways that favor ourselves, and then to seek evidence that shows we are excellent co-operators.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

200
Q

When husbands and wives estimate the percentage of housework each does, their estimates total more than 120 percent. 21.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

201
Q

When MBA students in a work group make estimates of their contributions to the team, the estimates total 139 percent. 22.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

202
Q

When subjects read the essay about self-serving biases and were then asked to write an essay about weaknesses in their own case, their previous righteousness was shaken. Subjects in this study were just as fair-minded as those who learned their identities at the last minute.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

203
Q

People were quite happy to learn about the various forms of self-serving bias and then apply their newfound knowledge to predict others’ responses. But their self-ratings were unaffected. Even when you grab people by the lapels, shake them, and say, “Listen to me! Most people have an inflated view of themselves. Be realistic!” they refuse,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

204
Q

“naive realism”: Each of us thinks we see the world directly, as it really is. We further believe that the facts as we see them are there for all to see, therefore others should agree with us. If they don’t agree, it follows either that they have not yet been exposed to the relevant facts or else that they are blinded by their interests and ideologies.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

205
Q

To the naive realist, that everyone is influenced by ideology and self-interest. Except for me. I see things as they are.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

206
Q

Good and evil do not exist outside of our beliefs about them.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

207
Q

With peace and harmony ascendant, Americans seemed to be searching for substitute villains.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

208
Q

Most of them share three properties: They are invisible (you can’t identify the evil one from appearance alone); their evil spreads by contagion, making it vital to protect impressionable young people from infection (for example from communist ideas, homosexual teachers, or stereotypes on television); and the villains can be defeated only if we all pull together as a team.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

209
Q

“Our life is the creation of our mind,” as Buddha said,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

210
Q

When taking the perpetrator’s perspective, he found that people who do things we see as evil, from spousal abuse all the way to genocide, rarely think they are doing anything wrong. They almost always see themselves as responding to attacks and provocations in ways that are justified. They often think that they themselves are victims.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

211
Q

“the myth of pure evil. ” Of this myth’s many parts, the most important are that evildoers are pure in their evil motives (they have no motives for their actions beyond sadism and greed); victims are pure in their victimhood (they did nothing to bring about their victimization); and evil comes from outside and is associated with a group or force that attacks our group. Furthermore, anyone who questions the application of the myth, who dares muddy the waters of moral certainty, is in league with evil.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

212
Q

Baumeister found that violence and cruelty have four main causes. The first two are obvious attributes of evil: greed/ambition (violence for direct personal gain, as in robbery) and sadism (pleasure in hurting people). But greed/ambition explains only a small portion of violence, and sadism explains almost none.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

213
Q

The two biggest causes of evil are two that we think are good, and that we try to encourage in our children: high self-esteem and moral idealism.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

214
Q

Threatened self-esteem accounts for a large portion of violence at the individual level, but to really get a mass atrocity going you need idealism—the belief that your violence is a means to a moral end.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

215
Q

Clifford Geertz wrote that “man is an animal suspended in webs of significance that he himself has spun. ”32.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

216
Q

The great lesson that comes out of ancient India is that life as we experience it is a game called “samsara. ” It is a game in which each person plays out his “dharma,” his role or part in a giant play.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

217
Q

If you want the truth to stand clear before you, never be for or against. The struggle between “for” and “against” is the mind’s worst disease. 34.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

218
Q

Judgmentalism is indeed a disease of the mind: it leads to anger, torment, and conflict. But it is also the mind’s normal condition—the elephant is always evaluating, always saying “Like it” or “Don’t like it. ”.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

219
Q

Once anger comes into play, people find it extremely difficult to empathize with and understand another perspective.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

220
Q

Debiasing occurred only when subjects were forced to look at themselves. ).

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

221
Q

When you first catch sight of a fault in yourself, you’ll likely hear frantic arguments from your inner lawyer excusing you and blaming others, but try not to listen.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

222
Q

Was really upset by X. But I guess I shouldn’t have done P, so I can see why you felt Q. ” Reciprocity amplified by self-serving biases drove you apart back when you were matching insults or hostile gestures, but you can turn the process around and use reciprocity to end a conflict and save a relationship. The human mind may have been shaped.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

223
Q

When pleasure or pain comes to them, the wise feel above pleasure and pain. —BUDDHA1.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

224
Q

Do not seek to have events happen as you want them to, but instead want them to happen as they do happen, and your life will go well. —EPICTETUS2.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

225
Q

Buddhism and Stoicism teach that striving for external goods, or to make the world conform to your wishes, is always a striving after wind. Happiness can only be found within, by breaking attachments to external things and cultivating an attitude of acceptance.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

226
Q

Battling the disappointment of success. The pleasure of getting what you want is often fleeting.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

227
Q

Here’s the trick with reinforcement: It works best when it comes seconds—not minutes or hours—after the behavior.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

228
Q

The elephant works the same way: It feels pleasure whenever it takes a step in the right direction. The elephant learns whenever pleasure (or pain) follows immediately after behavior, but it has trouble connecting success on Friday with actions it took on Monday.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

229
Q

“pre-goal attainment positive affect,” which is the pleasurable feeling you get as you make progress toward a goal. The second is called “post-goal attainment positive affect,” which Davidson says arises once you have achieved something you want. 3.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

230
Q

You experience this latter feeling as contentment, as a short-lived feeling of release when the left prefrontal cortex reduces its activity after a goal has been achieved. In other words, when it comes to goal pursuit, it really is the journey that counts, not the destination. Set for yourself any goal you want. Most of the pleasure will be had along the way, with every step that takes you closer.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

231
Q

Success is often no more thrilling than the relief of taking off a heavy backpack at the end of a long hike.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

232
Q

“the progress principle”: Pleasure comes more from making progress toward goals than from achieving them. Shakespeare captured it perfectly: “Things won are done; joy’s soul lies in the doing. ”4.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

233
Q

Of course, it’s better to win the lottery than to break your neck, but not by as much as you’d think. Because whatever happens, you’re likely to adapt to it, but you don’t realize up front that you will. We are bad at “affective forecasting,” 5 that is, predicting how we’ll feel in the future. We grossly overestimate the intensity and the duration of our emotional reactions. Within a year, lottery winners and paraplegics have both (on average) returned most of the way to their baseline levels of happiness. 6.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

234
Q

The human mind is extraordinarily sensitive to changes in conditions, but not so sensitive to absolute levels.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

235
Q

Forming lottery winner support groups to deal with their new difficulties. 7.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

236
Q

“My expectations were reduced to zero when I was twenty-one. Everything since then has been a bonus. ”8.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

237
Q

This is the adaptation principle at work: People’s judgments about their present state are based on whether it is better or worse than the state to which they have become accustomed. 9 Adaptation is, in part, just a property of neurons: Nerve cells respond vigorously to new stimuli, but gradually they “habituate,” firing less to stimuli that they have become used to.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

238
Q

Human beings, however, take adaptation to cognitive extremes.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

239
Q

We don’t just habituate, we recalibrate. We create for ourselves a world of targets, and each time we hit one we replace it with another. After a string of successes we aim higher; after a massive setback, such as a broken neck, we aim lower. Instead of following Buddhist and Stoic advice to surrender attachments and let events happen, we surround ourselves with goals, hopes, and expectations, and then feel pleasure and pain in relation to our progress. 10.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

240
Q

It doesn’t much matter what happens to you. Good fortune or bad, you will always return to your happiness setpoint—your brain’s default level of happiness—which was determined largely by your genes.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

241
Q

The Stoic philosophers of Ancient Greece, such as Epictetus, taught their followers to focus only on what they could fully control, which meant primarily their own thoughts and reactions.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

242
Q

The second biggest finding in happiness research, after the strong influence of genes upon a person’s average level of happiness, is that most environmental and demographic factors influence happiness very little.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

243
Q

A good marriage is one of the life-factors most strongly and consistently associated with happiness. 14 Part of this apparent benefit comes from “reverse correlation”: Happiness causes marriage.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

244
Q

Religious people are happier, on average, than nonreligious people. 17 This effect arises from the social ties that come with participation in a religious community, as well as from feeling connected to something beyond the self.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

245
Q

White Americans are freed from many of the hassles and indignities that affect black Americans, yet, on average, they are only very slightly happier. 18 Men have more freedom and power than women, yet they are not on average any happier. (Women experience more depression, but also more intense joy). 19.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

246
Q

Ed Diener,26 is that within any given country, at the lowest end of the income scale money does buy happiness: People who worry every day about paying for food and shelter report significantly less well-being than those who don’t. But once you are freed from basic needs and have entered the middle class, the relationship between wealth and happiness becomes smaller. The rich are happier on average than the middle class, but only by a little, and part of this relationship is reverse correlation: Happy people grow rich faster because, as in the marriage market, they are more appealing to others (such as bosses), and also because their frequent positive emotions help them to commit to projects, to work hard, and to invest in their futures. 27.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

247
Q

Wealth itself has only a small direct effect on happiness because it so effectively speeds up the hedonic treadmill.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

248
Q

One of the most consistent lessons the ancient sages teach is to let go, stop striving, and choose a new path. Turn inwards, or toward God, but for God’s sake stop trying to make the world conform to your will.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

249
Q

Maybe the only way to find happiness therefore is to change one’s own internal setting (for example, through meditation, Prozac, or cognitive therapy) instead of changing one’s environment?.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

250
Q

Yes, genes explain far more about us than anyone had realized, but the genes themselves often turn out to be sensitive to environmental conditions. 32 And yes, each person has a characteristic level of happiness, but it now looks as though it’s not so much a set point as a potential range or probability distribution. Whether you operate on the high or the low side of your potential range is determined by many factors that Buddha and Epictetus would have considered externals.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

251
Q

Martin Seligman founded positive psychology in the late 1990s,.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

252
Q

Two fundamentally different kinds of externals: the conditions of your life and the voluntary activities that you undertake. 33.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

253
Q

Voluntary activities, therefore, offer much greater promise for increasing happiness while avoiding adaptation effects.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

254
Q

“happiness formula:” H=S+C+V.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

255
Q

The level of happiness that you actually experience (H) is determined by your biological set point (S) plus the conditions of your life (C) plus the voluntary activities (V) you do. 34.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

256
Q

Research shows that people who must adapt to new and chronic sources of noise (such as when a new highway is built) never fully adapt, and even studies that find some adaptation still find evidence of impairment on cognitive tasks. Noise, especially noise that is variable or intermittent, interferes with concentration and increases stress. 35.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

257
Q

Although people quickly adapt to having more space,36 they don’t fully adapt to the longer commute, particularly if it involves driving in heavy traffic. 37 Even after years of commuting, those whose commutes are traffic-filled still arrive at work with higher levels of stress hormones. (Driving under ideal conditions is, however, often enjoyable and relaxing. )38.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

258
Q

None of these subjects pressed the button, yet the belief that they had some form of control made the noise less distressing to them.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

259
Q

On the floor with increased control, residents were happier, more active, and more alert (as rated by the nurses, not just by the residents), and these benefits were still visible eighteen months later. Most amazingly, at the eighteen-month follow-up, residents of the floor given control had better health and half as many deaths (15 percent versus 30 percent). 40.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

260
Q

Overall, attractive people are not happier than unattractive ones. Yet, surprisingly, some improvements in a person’s appearance do lead to lasting increases in happiness. 42.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

261
Q

Good relationships make people happy, and happy people enjoy more and better relationships than unhappy people. 44.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

262
Q

Conflicts in relationships—having an annoying office mate or room-mate, or having chronic conflict with your spouse—is one of the surest ways to reduce your happiness. You never adapt to interpersonal conflict;45 it damages every day, even days when you don’t see the other person but ruminate about the conflict nonetheless.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

263
Q

People who report the greatest interest in attaining money, fame, or beauty are consistently found to be less happy, and even less healthy, than those who pursue less materialistic goals. 47.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

264
Q

Mihalyi Csikszentmihalyi (pronounced “cheeks sent me high”),.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

265
Q

People report the highest levels of happiness, on average. People really enjoy eating, especially in the company of others, and they hate to be interrupted by telephone calls (and perhaps Csikszentmihalyi’s beeps) during meals, or (worst of all) during sex.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

266
Q

To continue eating or having sex beyond a certain level of satisfaction can lead to disgust. 49.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

267
Q

The keys to flow: There’s a clear challenge that fully engages your attention; you have the skills to meet the challenge; and you get immediate feedback about how you are doing at each step (the progress principle).

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

268
Q

In the flow experience, elephant and rider are in perfect harmony. The elephant (automatic processes) is doing most of the work, running smoothly through the forest, while the rider (conscious thought) is completely absorbed in looking out for problems and opportunities, helping wherever he can.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

269
Q

Pleasures are “delights that have clear sensory and strong emotional components,”50 such as may be derived from food, sex, backrubs, and cool breezes. Gratifications are activities that engage you fully, draw on your strengths, and allow you to lose self-consciousness. Gratifications can lead to flow.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

270
Q

Because the elephant has a tendency to overindulge, the rider needs to encourage it to get up and move on to another activity.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

271
Q

They derive a great deal more pleasure from their food by eating slowly and paying more attention to the food as they eat it. 51 Because they savor, they ultimately eat less.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

272
Q

The wise man “chooses not the greatest quantity of food but the most tasty. ”52.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

273
Q

When we enter a state of flow, hard work becomes effortless.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

274
Q

You can find out your strengths by taking an online test at www. Authentichappiness. Org.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

275
Q

People experienced longer-lasting improvements in mood from the kindness and gratitude activities than from those in which they indulged themselves.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt

276
Q

Even though people were most nervous about doing the kindness and gratitude activities, which required them to violate social norms and risk embarrassment, once they actually did the activities they felt better for the rest of the day.

A

The Happiness Hypothesis by Jonathan Haidt